I have a research review in progress. Until then...
I read an email the other day in which the writer said he was "pouring over the image." Unless the image is liquid-proof, this could be problematic...
Pour refers to what happens to liquids when you tip their containers. If you're focusing a lot of attention on something, you're poring over it.
CORRECT:
I pored over every detail of Dumbledore's conversations with Harry to figure out what was coming next.
It was pouring out when I left work.
INCORRECT:
I don't want to spend hours pouring over tax guidelines, trying to figure out what deductions I can take.
I pored the oil into the saucepan before adding the garlic.
Friday, January 29, 2010
Monday, January 25, 2010
Links: 1/25/10
Travis at Obesity Panacea has a good discussion on eliminating risk vs. minimizing risk, and the unintended consequences of trying to protect kids from getting hurt: No balls (or bikes, or skateboards) allowed
Mike Elgan at Computer World gives some updated cell phone etiquette rules.
Ammon Shea at the New York Times Magazine discusses whether texting might be changing the rules of spelling.
A research review at Science Daily describes how llama--yes, llama--proteins could be used to detect potent neurotoxins.
The Oatmeal gives his usual, unique take on a common grammar problem: How to use a semicolon.
Ed Yong at Not Exactly Rocket Science describes research indicating that echolocation in bats and whales may have been enabled by changes to the same gene.
Mike Elgan at Computer World gives some updated cell phone etiquette rules.
Ammon Shea at the New York Times Magazine discusses whether texting might be changing the rules of spelling.
A research review at Science Daily describes how llama--yes, llama--proteins could be used to detect potent neurotoxins.
The Oatmeal gives his usual, unique take on a common grammar problem: How to use a semicolon.
Ed Yong at Not Exactly Rocket Science describes research indicating that echolocation in bats and whales may have been enabled by changes to the same gene.
Thursday, January 21, 2010
New NSF-funded site on science education
I just got this update from a friend (via LinkedIn):
A new NSF-funded site has just launched. According to the release:
"[The site is] dedicated to cataloging best practices in media-based science education and getting the word out about--and discussing--innovative new media-based science education programs and concepts (where media includes both traditional and new media). The site is located at:
http://www.mediasciencelearning.com/
To celebrate its launch (and help get its discussion areas quickly up to critical mass), the grantees (at their own expense, not NSF's) are giving away Zingerman's gift certificates to the best (and most prolific) contributors to the site's case discussions, located at:
http://www.mediasciencelearning.com/CaseStudies
Related to this, if you know of any great media-based science education programs that ought to be highlighted by this site (or you run one yourself and are looking for feedback, publicity, collaborators, or funders), you can submit it at:
http://www.mediasciencelearning.com/addacase/
"
A new NSF-funded site has just launched. According to the release:
"[The site is] dedicated to cataloging best practices in media-based science education and getting the word out about--and discussing--innovative new media-based science education programs and concepts (where media includes both traditional and new media). The site is located at:
http://www.mediasciencelearning.com/
To celebrate its launch (and help get its discussion areas quickly up to critical mass), the grantees (at their own expense, not NSF's) are giving away Zingerman's gift certificates to the best (and most prolific) contributors to the site's case discussions, located at:
http://www.mediasciencelearning.com/CaseStudies
Related to this, if you know of any great media-based science education programs that ought to be highlighted by this site (or you run one yourself and are looking for feedback, publicity, collaborators, or funders), you can submit it at:
http://www.mediasciencelearning.com/addacase/
"
Labels:
biology,
education,
evolution,
genetics,
geology,
links: news,
links: pass it on,
links: resources,
physics,
science
Wednesday, January 20, 2010
Usage tip: sleight vs. slight
Today's tip comes to you courtesy of Tetley tea, who puts short quips on the paper tabs of their teabags. One I got today said "False friends give slight-of-handshakes."
Clever idea, but wrong word.
Slight means small, diminutive, thin, etc. Sleight means cunning, skill, or dexterity. So, false friends actually give sleight-of-handshakes.
CORRECT:
The magician used sleight-of-hand to hide the ball under the table.
We need to take a slight detour to avoid the construction.
INCORRECT:
Don't try to fool me with slight of hand!
At the next intersection, take a sleight right.
Clever idea, but wrong word.
Slight means small, diminutive, thin, etc. Sleight means cunning, skill, or dexterity. So, false friends actually give sleight-of-handshakes.
CORRECT:
The magician used sleight-of-hand to hide the ball under the table.
We need to take a slight detour to avoid the construction.
INCORRECT:
Don't try to fool me with slight of hand!
At the next intersection, take a sleight right.
Tuesday, January 19, 2010
More evidence that simple classroom interventions can have positive effects on achievement
Reading the article I summarized yesterday reminded me of a pair of articles I read recently on a related topic. The first was published in the 1 September 2006 issue of Science, and the second was published in the 17 April 2009 issue. Both describe the results of affirmation exercises on student achievement.
For the first article, Cohen et al. studied two groups of seventh graders (approximately 120 students per group), about half European-American and half African-American. They randomly assigned students to one of two groups (an experimental group or a control group). Students in both groups were given short (~1 paragraph) writing assignments. Students in the experimental group were instructed to choose and write about values that were important to them; those in the control group wrote about values that were unimportant to them.
The intervention occurred at the beginning of the fall semester during the experimental year. Researchers followed up on students' fall semester grades to evaluate the effectiveness of the intervention.
In the second article, the researchers describe the results from a third experiment (a third set of students given the assignment), plus additional data on the students from the original groups, two years later.
This is where it gets interesting. As you read the following, keep in mind that this intervention took about 15 minutes and involved no special equipment, no curriculum modification, and no teacher preparation at all.
African-American students in the treatment group showed significant increases in GPA (~0.24 points) compared to their peers in the control group. African-American students with the lowest achievement going into the class (and the lowest expectation for achievement in the class) had an increase in GPA of 0.41 points compared to their peers in the control group. These effects were present even two years later. What's more, students in the treatment group were less likely to be held back or assigned to remedial classes. Students in the treatment groups also reported more positive self-perceptions, even over the long term.
European-American students showed no differences between control and experimental groups. In fact, the treatment reduced the "achievement gap" (the observed difference in performance between African-American students and their white classmates) by approximately 40%.
The researchers hypothesize that the affirmation exercises act to break a recursive cycle of failure. To quote, "A feedback loop, with psychological threat and poor performance reinforcing one another, can create worsening performance over time. Students' poor performance may also cause them to be seen as less able by their teachers and less worthy of attention and mentoring, increasing the likelihood of lower performance." In other words, students who fail early--and therefore assume that they are less capable--have lower expectations of achievement. Those low expectations can become a self-fulfilling prophecy: students expect to do poorly, so they do.
Because the affirmation exercises focus students on things they value, the exercises can increase students' feelings of self-worth. By reinforcing students' self-esteem, the exercises help to break students out of the failure feedback loop.
The differential effects of the treatment on African-American and European-American students may arise because of the so-called "stereotype threat." A common stereotype is that African-American (and other minority) students perform poorly compared to white students. Therefore, minority students are more likely to fall into a feedback loop when confronted with failure--students' awareness of the stereotype makes them more conscious of their performance, and if they do poorly, they think they're conforming to the stereotype, which puts them under more pressure, which makes them do even more poorly...you get the idea. Even minority students who perform well may experience negative stereotyping effects--their awareness of the stereotype (again) puts pressure on them, which negatively affects performance.
The researchers hypothesize that doing the affirmation exercises helps to reduce the psychological "threat" of stereotyping, allowing students to perform better.
Together, these two papers provide additional evidence that even short-term, simple interventions can make significant strides in closing the achievement gap between white and minority students. As in the article I reviewed yesterday, the effects of this simple exercise were most pronounced for the most at-risk students (low-achieving African-American students), and the exercise had no downside for high-performing students. The results also suggest that such interventions might have long-lasting positive effects. Obviously, these kinds of exercises will not solve all of the educational issues facing the country--but they are cheap (in terms of both time and money), easy, and effective.
(And by the way, the older article is available--full text--for free online at www.sciencemag.org.)
Cohen, G., Garcia, J., Purdie-Vaughns, V., Apfel, N., & Brzustoski, P. (2009). Recursive Processes in Self-Affirmation: Intervening to Close the Minority Achievement Gap Science, 324 (5925), 400-403 DOI: 10.1126/science.1170769
Cohen, G. (2006). Reducing the Racial Achievement Gap: A Social-Psychological Intervention Science, 313 (5791), 1307-1310 DOI: 10.1126/science.1128317
Monday, January 18, 2010
Things I learned at ScienceOnline2010
Might as well jump on the bandwagon...
10.5 Things I learned at ScienceOnline2010:
1. Science bloggers, and people who willingly spend an entire weekend in RTP with them, are pretty amazingly cool people.
2. There's too much attention paid to failing kids, and not enough to the awesome ones.
3. In all the huffle about NCLB and K-12 STEM education, we may have overlooked an important group who are just as excited and interested in science: adults (and here).
4. Food on ocean research vessels is awesome. (Who knew?)
5. Fact-checking isn't just making sure you got the science right. Oh, and it's not always clear what constitutes a "fact."
6. Minorities are edging out whites in the use of handheld devices.
7. Apparently, college students no longer check their email.
8. Google is working on a brain implant, so you can be constantly online.
9. Working online and using social media can allow people to be judged more by their thoughts than their appearance. But that might not necessarily be a good thing.
10. The North Carolina Museum of Natural Sciences is very large. And, it hosts an insect festival every year in which one can consume stir-fried crickets and meal worm pie.
10.5. The Sleep Number Bed is the world's most expensive air mattress...and that's about all it is.
10.5 Things I learned at ScienceOnline2010:
1. Science bloggers, and people who willingly spend an entire weekend in RTP with them, are pretty amazingly cool people.
2. There's too much attention paid to failing kids, and not enough to the awesome ones.
3. In all the huffle about NCLB and K-12 STEM education, we may have overlooked an important group who are just as excited and interested in science: adults (and here).
4. Food on ocean research vessels is awesome. (Who knew?)
5. Fact-checking isn't just making sure you got the science right. Oh, and it's not always clear what constitutes a "fact."
6. Minorities are edging out whites in the use of handheld devices.
7. Apparently, college students no longer check their email.
8. Google is working on a brain implant, so you can be constantly online.
9. Working online and using social media can allow people to be judged more by their thoughts than their appearance. But that might not necessarily be a good thing.
10. The North Carolina Museum of Natural Sciences is very large. And, it hosts an insect festival every year in which one can consume stir-fried crickets and meal worm pie.
10.5. The Sleep Number Bed is the world's most expensive air mattress...and that's about all it is.
Focusing students' attention on the relevance of science improves educational outcomes
In the discussion about improving science education, the focus often falls on large-scale programs. However, some studies are suggesting that even short-term, small interventions in classrooms can have significant effects on student achievement. In the 4 December 2009 issue of Science (okay, I'm a little behind in my reading...), Hulleman and Harackiewicz investigate the efficacy of short writing assignments on student interest and achievement in science.
It's generally well accepted (and logical) that students with an interest or stake in the material they are learning perform better, work harder, and retain more of what they learn. However, it can be difficult to identify causal relationships between various interventions and student success, because many programs that do produce good results include a lot of different components, not all of which are focused on increasing student interest in the topic.
Hulleman and Harackiewicz hypothesized that activities that draw students' attention to the relevance of instruction to their everyday lives would lead to an increase in achievement. They also hypothesized that the increase in achievement would be largest among students who started out with low expectations of their own abilities--that is, students who think they will do poorly in science would benefit more from these interventions than students who already think they will do well.
To test these hypotheses, the researchers performed an experiment involving 262 high school students. The students attended two different high schools, were taught by seven different teachers, and were enrolled in three different science courses (biology, physical science, or integrated science). Nearly all (92%) of the students were ninth-graders (the remaining 8% were tenth-graders). About half (52%) were female. The majority (66%) were white; 15% were African-American, 12% were Asian, and 8% were Hispanic. The experiment spanned a single academic semester, but data were also collected on about 40% (100/262) of the students' grades for the following semester.
The students were randomly divided into two groups (one experimental group and one control group). Slightly more than half (136/262) of the students were in the experimental group. The teachers did not know which group each student had been assigned to. Each group was assigned to write between one and eight short essays over the course of the semester. Students in the control group wrote summaries of the content they were learning in the class. Students in the experimental group wrote essays describing the relevance of what they were learning to their own lives. (It was not clear whether that context was provided to the students or whether they were expected to identify it themselves.)
At the beginning of the semester (before the experiment), the researchers surveyed students to learn their expectations of success in the course and their interest in science. They also surveyed the students at the end of the semester to reassess their level of interest in science and to learn their future plans for careers in scientific fields. Students' grades during the semester were also monitored.
The results suggest that even this type of simple, short-term intervention can significantly improve student results, at least for those students who start the class with low expectations. Students with low expectations who were in the experimental group had significantly higher grades--"nearly two-thirds of a letter grade", to be exact--than those in the control group. Low-expectation students in the experimental group also showed significantly increased interest in science compared to those in the control group.
Among students with high initial performance expectations, there were no statistically significant performance or interest increases from the intervention. In other words, students who thought they'd do well in the class generally had the same grades (and post-class interest in science), regardless of which group they were in.
Although these results are encouraging--they suggest that teachers can increase the likelihood of student success, especially for the most at-risk students, through a few simple writing assignments--the sample size was small, and relatively homogeneous. Larger studies involving more students, especially minority students, would increase the reliability of the results. Until then, though, these results certainly suggest that these kinds of assignments can't hurt, and could be very helpful.
Hulleman, C., & Harackiewicz, J. (2009). Promoting Interest and Performance in High School Science Classes Science, 326 (5958), 1410-1412 DOI: 10.1126/science.1177067
Sunday, January 17, 2010
Some more useful science online resources
Scivee--share your science online!
SciNet--a new social network for scientists of all stripes, from AAAS.
Periodic Table--interactive periodic table. h/t Staten Island Academy student in ScienceOnline session. (Play with the temperature slider and see how the states of different elements change!)
Miss Baker's Biology Class--some really amazing student projects, blogs, etc.
Staten Island Academy's blog community--a great role model for student blogging.
SciNet--a new social network for scientists of all stripes, from AAAS.
Periodic Table--interactive periodic table. h/t Staten Island Academy student in ScienceOnline session. (Play with the temperature slider and see how the states of different elements change!)
Miss Baker's Biology Class--some really amazing student projects, blogs, etc.
Staten Island Academy's blog community--a great role model for student blogging.
Labels:
#scio10,
biology,
evolution,
genetics,
geology,
links: pass it on,
links: resources,
physics,
science
Usage tip: affect vs. effect
In nearly all cases, affect is a verb (meaning "to have an influence on"), and effect is a noun (meaning "result" or "influence"). Thus:
CORRECT
That movie had a strong effect on me.
The news report affected me greatly.
How does gravity affect matter?
What are the effects of solar radiation?
INCORRECT
The book I just read really effected me.
I don't drink because I don't like the affects.
Can you describe some of the affects of commercials?
Will Internet usage effect how we learn information?
One way I often remember it is this: Just like a comes before e in the alphabet, you have to affect something before you see an effect.
Just to confuse things, of course, there are situations in which affect is a noun, and in which effect is a verb. However, those usages are not parallel like the ones above.
Affect as a noun is almost exclusively used in psychology (and by people who are not in psychology but who want to sound like they are). In that context, affect refers to a person's behavior, mood, and general emotional state. (Also in this context, the word is pronounce "A-fekt", not "a-FEKT".)
Effect as a verb means "to put into motion" or "to cause". In this context, the word is pronounced "ee-FEKT", as opposed to the more common "uh-FEKT". It's almost always (at least in my experience) used with "change," as in "to effect a change". Again, this is an uncommon usage.
In general, to figure out whether to use affect or effect, try replacing the word with another noun or verb. If you can replace it with a verb, use affect. If you can replace it with a noun, use effect. But please, whatever you do, do not use impact!!
CORRECT
That movie had a strong effect on me.
The news report affected me greatly.
How does gravity affect matter?
What are the effects of solar radiation?
INCORRECT
The book I just read really effected me.
I don't drink because I don't like the affects.
Can you describe some of the affects of commercials?
Will Internet usage effect how we learn information?
One way I often remember it is this: Just like a comes before e in the alphabet, you have to affect something before you see an effect.
Just to confuse things, of course, there are situations in which affect is a noun, and in which effect is a verb. However, those usages are not parallel like the ones above.
Affect as a noun is almost exclusively used in psychology (and by people who are not in psychology but who want to sound like they are). In that context, affect refers to a person's behavior, mood, and general emotional state. (Also in this context, the word is pronounce "A-fekt", not "a-FEKT".)
Effect as a verb means "to put into motion" or "to cause". In this context, the word is pronounced "ee-FEKT", as opposed to the more common "uh-FEKT". It's almost always (at least in my experience) used with "change," as in "to effect a change". Again, this is an uncommon usage.
In general, to figure out whether to use affect or effect, try replacing the word with another noun or verb. If you can replace it with a verb, use affect. If you can replace it with a noun, use effect. But please, whatever you do, do not use impact!!
Saturday, January 16, 2010
Some cool science and education web sites, courtesy of ScienceOnline2010
FieldTripEarth--free data, plus info from real, live research programs going on all over the world.
NESCent--the National Evolutionary Synthesis Center. More (lots more) free data, plus educational materials on cutting-edge evolution research. (I think when the speaker demonstrated this, half the room started drooling at the data that are available...)
EduWeb--a source for online/digital, educational games on science, technology, history, and art.
Dryad--another open access data depository.
PRI's The World Science--weekly science podcasts, news stories, etc.
Scitopia--a peer-reviewed-research search engine.
Fold.it--play computer games for SCIENCE!
Spot.us--donate to journalists needing funding to cover their stories.
Science for Citizens--find research projects looking for volunteers.
Pandemic II (game)--play the bad guy. Design germs, infect people, get points!
Science Cheerleaders--learn basic science facts, find citizen science projects, and take a brain makeover quiz!
NESCent--the National Evolutionary Synthesis Center. More (lots more) free data, plus educational materials on cutting-edge evolution research. (I think when the speaker demonstrated this, half the room started drooling at the data that are available...)
EduWeb--a source for online/digital, educational games on science, technology, history, and art.
Dryad--another open access data depository.
PRI's The World Science--weekly science podcasts, news stories, etc.
Scitopia--a peer-reviewed-research search engine.
Fold.it--play computer games for SCIENCE!
Spot.us--donate to journalists needing funding to cover their stories.
Science for Citizens--find research projects looking for volunteers.
Pandemic II (game)--play the bad guy. Design germs, infect people, get points!
Science Cheerleaders--learn basic science facts, find citizen science projects, and take a brain makeover quiz!
Labels:
#scio10,
biology,
evolution,
genetics,
geology,
links: pass it on,
links: resources,
origin of life,
physics,
random stuff,
science
Subscribe to:
Posts (Atom)